The purpose of our study was to investigate whether subliminal evaluative conditioning could change implicit and explicit depression-related cognition. Subliminal evaluative conditioning was conducted as a form of Primed Lexical Decision Task, in which subliminally presented self-related word was followed supraminally presented positive words. For measuring implicit depression-related cognition, we used Depression Implicit Association Task (Depression IAT), and for explicit depression-related cognition, we used Depression subscale extracting from Depression And Anxiety Cognition Scale (DACS-D). Furthermore, we also investigated whether the changes of implicit and explicit cognition could be last for 24 hours. As a result, we found that evaluative conditioning changed the implicit depression-related cognition. However, this changing in implicit cognition did not last for 24 hours later. In explicit depression-related cognition, evaluative conditioning by PLDT changed not all subscale but negative automatic thought for self. These results suggested that evaluative conditioning made temporally changing to implicit depression-related cognition while sustained changing to explicit depressive thought for self.
Published in | American Journal of Applied Psychology (Volume 6, Issue 2) |
DOI | 10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11 |
Page(s) | 15-21 |
Creative Commons |
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited. |
Copyright |
Copyright © The Author(s), 2017. Published by Science Publishing Group |
IAT, Evaluative Conditioning, Depression, Implicit Depressive Cognition
[1] | Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press |
[2] | Coyne, J., & Gotlib, I. (1983). The role of cognition in depression: A critical appraisal. Psychological bulletin, 94, 472–505. |
[3] | Murrough, J. W., Iacoviello, B., Neumeister, A., Charney, D. S., & Iosifescu, D. V. (2011). Cognitive dysfunction in depression: neurocircuitry and new therapeutic strategies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 96(4), 553–63. |
[4] | Wiers, R. W., Teachman, B. A., & De Houwer. (2007). Implicit cognitive processes in psychopathology: An introduction. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry.38, 95-104. |
[5] | Gotlib, I. H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and Depression: Current Status and Future Directions. Annual review of Clinical Psychology, 27, 285–312. |
[6] | Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4-27. |
[7] | Monteith, L., & Pettit, J. W. (2011). Implicit and explicit stigmatizing attitudes and stereotypes about depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30, 484–505. |
[8] | Aikawa, A. & Fujii, T. (2011). Using the Implicit Association Test (IAT) to measure implicit shyness. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 82, 41-48. |
[9] | Teachman, B. A., Marker, C. D., & Smith-janik, S. B. (2009). Change over the course of treatment. Jounal of Conslulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 988–1002. |
[10] | Franck, E., Raedt, R. D., & Houwer, J. D. (2008). Activation of latent self-schemas as a cognitive vulnerability factor for depression: The potential role of implicit self-esteem. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1588-1599. |
[11] | De Raedt, R., Schacht, R., Franck, E., & De Hower, J. (2006). Self-esteem and depression revisited: Implicit positive self-esteem in depressed patients? Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 1017-1028. |
[12] | Meites, T. M., Deveney, C. M., Steele, K. T., Holmes, A. J., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2008). Implicit depression and hopelessness in remitted depressed individuals. Behavior Research and Therapy, 46, 1078-1084. |
[13] | Teachman, B. a., & Woody, S. R. (2003). Automatic processing in spider phobia: Implicit fear associations over the course of treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(1), 100–109. |
[14] | Ozaki, Y. (2006). Changing implicit attitudes by repeated approach or avoidance. The Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 98-110. |
[15] | Gast, A., De Houwer, J., & De Schryver, M. (2012). Evaluative conditioning can be modulated by memory of the CS–US pairings at the time of testing. Learning and Motivation, 43(3), 116–126. |
[16] | Dijkserhuis, A. (2004). I like myself but I don’t know why: Enhancing Implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 345-355. |
[17] | Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, j., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000) Just say no (to Stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 871-888. |
[18] | Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692-731. |
[19] | Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Austin, A. J., & Cox, W. T. L. (2012). Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice habit-breaking intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 1267–1278. |
[20] | Lemmens, L. H. J. M., Roefs, A., Arntz, A., van Teeseling, H. C., Peeters, F., & Huibers, M. J. H. (2014). The value of an implicit self-associative measure specific to core beliefs of depression. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45(1), 196–202. |
[21] | De Raedt, R., Schacht, R., Franck, E., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Self-esteem and depression revisited: implicit positive self-esteem in depressed patients? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(7), 1017–28. |
[22] | Grano, C., Muzi, M., Pompili, M., Erbuto, E., & Violani, C. (2016). Measuring the automatic negative self-schema: New evidence for the construct and criterion validity of the Depression Implicit Association. Self and Identity, 15(5), 599-613. |
[23] | Creemers, D. H. M., Scholte, R. H. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., Pieters, S., & Wiers, R. W. (2013). Acute stress increases implicit depression and decreases implicit self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 4, 118-132. |
[24] | Rinck, M. (2016). CBM research needs more power: Commentary on the special issue on cognitive bias modification. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. |
[25] | Friedman, M. A., Nosek, B. A., Miller, I. W., Gordon, K. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit hopelessness and severity of depressive symptoms. Unpublished manuscript. |
[26] | Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, A. B., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: I. An Improved Scoring Algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197-216. |
[27] | Fuiji, T., Ikeda, R., Uebuchi, H. (2009). The priming effect on the achievement motivation. Bulletin of Tokyo Gakugei University. Educational sciences, 60, 131-139. |
[28] | Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self- concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022-1038. |
[29] | Fukui, I. (1998). Development of depression and anxiety cognition scale: toward the construction of a cognitive-behavioral model of depression and anxiety. Japanese journal of behavior therapy, 24, 57-70. |
[30] | Grumm, M., Nestler, S., & Collani, G. Von. (2009). Changing explicit and implicit attitudes: The case of self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 327-335. |
[31] | Roefs, A., Hujiding, J., Smulders, F. T. Y., MacLeod, C. M., Jong, P, J., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. T. M. (2011). Implicit measures of association in psychopathology research. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 149-193. |
[32] | De Raedt, R. (2006). Activation of implicit self-schemas and a difficulty to disengage from negative cognitions in depression: An experimental psychopathology approach. Psychological Belgica, 46, 117-130. |
[33] | Gawronski, B., & Strack, F. (2004). On the propositional nature of cognitive consistency:Dissonance changes explicit, but not implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 535–542. |
[34] | Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 275-287. |
[35] | Peters, K. R., & Gawronski, B. (2011). Mutual influences between the implicit and explicit self-concepts: The role of memory activation and motivated reasoning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 436–442. |
[36] | Hertel, P. T., & Mathews, A. (2011). Cognitive Bias Modification: Past Perspectives, Current Findings, and Future Applications. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 521-536. |
[37] | Hallion, L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychological Bulletin, 137(6), 940–958. |
APA Style
Akihiro Masuyama, Kengo Yokomitsu, Yuji Sakano. (2017). Subliminal Evaluative Conditioning Changed Implicit and Explicit Depressive Cognition. American Journal of Applied Psychology, 6(2), 15-21. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11
ACS Style
Akihiro Masuyama; Kengo Yokomitsu; Yuji Sakano. Subliminal Evaluative Conditioning Changed Implicit and Explicit Depressive Cognition. Am. J. Appl. Psychol. 2017, 6(2), 15-21. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11
AMA Style
Akihiro Masuyama, Kengo Yokomitsu, Yuji Sakano. Subliminal Evaluative Conditioning Changed Implicit and Explicit Depressive Cognition. Am J Appl Psychol. 2017;6(2):15-21. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11
@article{10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11, author = {Akihiro Masuyama and Kengo Yokomitsu and Yuji Sakano}, title = {Subliminal Evaluative Conditioning Changed Implicit and Explicit Depressive Cognition}, journal = {American Journal of Applied Psychology}, volume = {6}, number = {2}, pages = {15-21}, doi = {10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11}, url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11}, eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.ajap.20170602.11}, abstract = {The purpose of our study was to investigate whether subliminal evaluative conditioning could change implicit and explicit depression-related cognition. Subliminal evaluative conditioning was conducted as a form of Primed Lexical Decision Task, in which subliminally presented self-related word was followed supraminally presented positive words. For measuring implicit depression-related cognition, we used Depression Implicit Association Task (Depression IAT), and for explicit depression-related cognition, we used Depression subscale extracting from Depression And Anxiety Cognition Scale (DACS-D). Furthermore, we also investigated whether the changes of implicit and explicit cognition could be last for 24 hours. As a result, we found that evaluative conditioning changed the implicit depression-related cognition. However, this changing in implicit cognition did not last for 24 hours later. In explicit depression-related cognition, evaluative conditioning by PLDT changed not all subscale but negative automatic thought for self. These results suggested that evaluative conditioning made temporally changing to implicit depression-related cognition while sustained changing to explicit depressive thought for self.}, year = {2017} }
TY - JOUR T1 - Subliminal Evaluative Conditioning Changed Implicit and Explicit Depressive Cognition AU - Akihiro Masuyama AU - Kengo Yokomitsu AU - Yuji Sakano Y1 - 2017/03/10 PY - 2017 N1 - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11 DO - 10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11 T2 - American Journal of Applied Psychology JF - American Journal of Applied Psychology JO - American Journal of Applied Psychology SP - 15 EP - 21 PB - Science Publishing Group SN - 2328-5672 UR - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20170602.11 AB - The purpose of our study was to investigate whether subliminal evaluative conditioning could change implicit and explicit depression-related cognition. Subliminal evaluative conditioning was conducted as a form of Primed Lexical Decision Task, in which subliminally presented self-related word was followed supraminally presented positive words. For measuring implicit depression-related cognition, we used Depression Implicit Association Task (Depression IAT), and for explicit depression-related cognition, we used Depression subscale extracting from Depression And Anxiety Cognition Scale (DACS-D). Furthermore, we also investigated whether the changes of implicit and explicit cognition could be last for 24 hours. As a result, we found that evaluative conditioning changed the implicit depression-related cognition. However, this changing in implicit cognition did not last for 24 hours later. In explicit depression-related cognition, evaluative conditioning by PLDT changed not all subscale but negative automatic thought for self. These results suggested that evaluative conditioning made temporally changing to implicit depression-related cognition while sustained changing to explicit depressive thought for self. VL - 6 IS - 2 ER -